Article Navigation
August 2003
Article Contents
-
Abstract
-
INTRODUCTION
-
VISUAL ECOLOGY: INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN ORGANISM AND ITS ENVIRONMENT MEDIATED BY VISUAL INFORMATION
-
CUBOMEDUSAE POSSESS THREE DISTINCT TYPES OF EYES
-
NERVOUS SYSTEM ORGANIZATION
-
PREVIOUS IDEAS FOR THE ROLE OF THE COMPLEX EYES IN A BOX JELLYFISH
-
THE LIFE HISTORY AND ECOLOGY OF THE CUBOMEDUSAE DIFFERS FROM OTHER MEDUSAE
-
DISCUSSION
-
SUPPOSITION: SPATIAL VISION ALLOWS FOR EXPLOITATION OF PRODUCTIVE NEAR-SHORE HABITATS BY CUBOZOANS
-
References
- < Previous
- Next >
Journal Article
Melissa M. Coates
Integrative and Comparative Biology, Volume 43, Issue 4, August 2003, Pages 542–548, https://doi.org/10.1093/icb/43.4.542
Published:
01 August 2003
- Split View
- Views
- Article contents
- Figures & tables
- Video
- Audio
- Supplementary Data
-
Cite
Cite
Melissa M. Coates, Visual Ecology and Functional Morphology of Cubozoa (Cnidaria), Integrative and Comparative Biology, Volume 43, Issue 4, August 2003, Pages 542–548, https://doi.org/10.1093/icb/43.4.542
Close
Search
Close
Search
Advanced Search
Search Menu
Abstract
Jellyfish belong to one of the oldest extant animal phyla, the Cnidaria. The first Cnidaria appear in the fossil record 600 million years ago, preceeding the Cambrian explosion. They are an extremely successful group present in all marine environments and some freshwater environments. In contrast to many animal phyla in which vision is a primary sense Cnidarians do not, generally, employ image forming eyes. One small class stands alone: the Cubozoa. Cubomedusae are commonly known as box jellyfish. They possess image forming eyes (Coates et al., 2001) which certainly evolved independently from other metazoans. Cubomedusae therefore offer a unique perspective on the evolution of image forming eyes. This literature review collects, into one place, what is known about: the multiple eye types of box jellyfish, cubomedusan life history and ecology, and the sensory and neural systems of box jellyfish. Here I discuss how these features set cubomedusae apart from scyphomedusae and hydromedusae. Knowledge in these areas is sparse; the work done to date inspires increased efforts.
INTRODUCTION
The class Cubozoa (Werner, 1975) is the smallest class within the phylum Cnidaria with only: one order (Cubomedusae), two families (Carybdeidae and Chiropdropidae), seven genera, and roughly two dozen species. In spite of this small size there is a lack of consensus on the naming and description of species. As a result the reader will notice different spellings of genera, for example Charybdea xaymacana (Conant, 1898) vs.Carybdea sivickisi (Hoverd, 1985) depending on the choice of the author who described that species. For this review I have chosen to hold to the spellings of the original authors.
Originally grouped as an order within the class Scyphozoa, the Cubozoa were elevated to class status once research revealed the magnitude of the differences between this and other cnidarian groups (Werner, 1973; Werner et al., 1976). Berger (1900) first noticed the physiological intermediacy of the cubomedusae between the scyphomedusae and hydromedusae. Although they share traits with the scyphozoans (rhopalia that come in multiples of four) and hydrozoans (possession of a nerve-ring), cubozoans also possess traits found in neither of the two other medusoid classes. These include complete metamorphosis from polyp to medusa (Werner, 1971, 1973; Arneson and Cutress, 1976; Laska-Mehnert, 1985), box-shaped exumbrella (in most species, Tripedalia cystophora being a notable exception), and, complex lensed camera-type eyes (Berger, 1898).
The eyes are located on the rhopalia, marginal sense organs which hang from the bell on stalks and are weighted down by a statolith. The function of the statolith is not known and it has been proposed as both a gravity-sensing organ and as a weight to keep the eyes oriented in the same direction regardless of body orientation (Berger, 1900). On each rhopalium there are two lensed eyes (large complex eye and small complex eye), an identical pair of pit-shaped ocelli (pit eyes), and an identical pair of slit-shaped ocelli (slit eyes) (Fig. 1A). There is one rhopalium on each side of the box-shaped bell, four in all. These alternate with a single tentacle, or a group of tentacles, at each corner of the bell (Fig. 1B). This makes four rhopalia per medusa, six eyes per rhopalium, a total of twenty-four eyes. What are the cubomedusae doing with all this visual hardware?
Nilsson and Pelger (1994) predict that eye evolution responds rapidly to lifestyle changes (see also Darwin, 1872). This implies that visual needs shape eye design much more than phylogenetic position. Therefore, it is useful to look at lifestyle differences between the cubozoans and other cnidarians and to ask: what is the need for complex eyes? Is this need driven by ecological imperatives unique to cubomedusae (among Cnidaria)?
VISUAL ECOLOGY: INTERACTIONS BETWEEN AN ORGANISM AND ITS ENVIRONMENT MEDIATED BY VISUAL INFORMATION
Ecology is the study of interactions of animals with their environment. While this is often the study of energy or nutrients moving through a system, information is an equally important component of the environment. Information is collected, dispersed, processed, created, and hidden by organisms in countless ways. The question becomes: with what sensory information do cubomedusae interact with their environment? How could that information enable the cubomedusae to succeed by encoding cues about location, habitat, obstacles, foods, mates, and so forth? Compared with the open ocean, kelp forests, mangroves, and coral reefs are similar near-shore environments in terms of obstacles in the water column. These obstacles all present low frequency visual information. These near-shore environments are the prefered habitat of cubozoan species (Table 1).
Cubomedusae exhibit many light mediated behaviors (Conant, 1898; Berger, 1900; Barnes, 1966; Hartwick, 1991; Hamner et al., 1995; Matsumoto, 1996; Stewart, 1996; Coates and Thompson, 2000). For example, Chironex fleckeri avoids dark obstacles in a tank and in the wild (Hamner et al., 1995) and can also orient to the light of a match (Barnes, 1966). Tripedalia cystophora rapidly swims away from dark objects and is attracted to light shafts in the water (personal observation; Coates and Thompson, 2000). Presumably the eyes of the sensory clubs mediate these behaviors based on the visual information they extract from the environment.
CUBOMEDUSAE POSSESS THREE DISTINCT TYPES OF EYES
Cubomedusae have six eyes located on each of the four rhopalia. The eyes can be separated into three distinct types: camera-type eyes, slit shaped ocelli, and pit shaped ocelli (Fig. 1A). All three eye types possess ciliated photoreceptors (Yamasu and Yoshida, 1976; Laska and Hündgen, 1982). Although the earliest reports of cubomedusae all mention these three eye types, often the number of ocelli present ranged from one to several (Haeckel, 1879; Agassiz and Mayer, 1902). Haake (1887, as reported by Berger [1900]) claimed that the simple lateral ocelli (pit and slit eyes) were present in young Charybdea rastonii, but absent in the adults. We know this to be incorrect from more recent descriptions of this species; the ocelli are present in both the young and the adults (Matsumoto, 1996). In fact all species on which current research has focused (Carybdea sp.—Satterlie, 1979; Mackie, 1999; Tripedalia cystophor—Laska and Hündgen, 1982; Stewart, 1996; Coates and Thompson, 2000; Chironex fleckeri—Hamner et al., 1995; Carybdea rastonii—Matsumoto, 1996) have the six eyes (two of each type) described here.
The ocelli
The ocelli come in two shapes, pits and slits, and are paired symmetrical to the medial line of the rhopalium (Fig. 1A). The ocelli are formed by invagination of the surface epithelium (Conant, 1898). The distal portions of the epithelial cells lining the invaginations are heavily pigmented (Berger, 1900; Ng, 1974). Berger (1900) and Conant (1898) found only one type of cell in these invaginations (as did Laska and Hündgen, 1982) while they mention that earlier authors (Schewiakoff, 1889; Claus, 1878) found two. The invaginated cells are photoreceptors (Yamasu and Yoshida, 1976). There is a refracting secretion in the space of this invagination presumably protecting the photoreceptor cells (Conant, 1898; Berger, 1900; Yamasu and Yoshida, 1976).
The camera-type eyes are similar in morphology to those of vertebrates and cephalopods
The complex eyes are made up of a cornea, cellular lens, possibly a vitreous space (see below) and a retina with pigmented cells (Conant, 1898; Berger, 1898). The similar morphology to that of other camera-type eyes, vertebrates and cephalopods, suggests a similar function (Pearse and Pearse, 1978). Apart from size differences, the large and small complex eyes are similar in morphology. In Carybdea marsupialis the large eye is 350–400 μm in diameter while the small eye has a diameter of 250–300 μm. The lens diameter is 150 μm in both eyes (Martin, 2002). Although there is some variation in eye size between cubozan species, the complex eyes are always roughly on this same order of magnitude.
The cornea is made up of flattened cells continuous with the epithelium of the rhopalium (Berger, 1898). All authors seem to be in agreement with this interpretation. The lens is cellular but the ‘interior lacks nuclei, cell walls, and protoplasmic structure’ (Berger, 1900). Berger also noted that the degree of cellularization of the interior of the lens varied with lens size, smaller lenses being more cellular. This difference holds between small and large eyes within a specimen, and between eyes of smaller and larger (younger and older) specimens. The lens is ectodermal in origin (Berger, 1900) and biconvex and heterogeneous. Mackie (1999) assumes this is a graded index of refraction lens. The crystallin proteins are novel (Piatigorsky et al., 1989, 1993). The lens is surrounded closely by a capsule which may be a secretion from the lens cells (Berger, 1900). Ng (1974) refers to this capsule as a vitreous humor, believing its position and function synonymous to that of the vertebrate eye.
Earlier authors report the presence of a large vitreous body beteween the lens and the retina (Schewiakoff, 1889; Conant, 1898). Conant (1898) reported what he termed ‘prism cells' in this body which possess a central fiber and concluded that they were extensions of the retinal cells. Berger (1900) clarifies this by proposing that the retina and the vitreous body are histologically different parts of the same structure (see also Laska and Hündgen, 1982). The term retina is thus used for both taken together, while vitreous body refers only to the vitreous portion of the retina. It is important to note that this implies there is no space between the lens and the retina, with the receptive segments of the photoreceptors extending all the way to the lens, as suggested by more recent authors (Laska and Hündgen, 1982; Pearse and Pearse, 1978; Satterlie, 2002). In this way Berger divided the retinal cells into three topographical regions or zones: the vitreous zone (vitreous body of other authors), the pigmented zone, and the nuclear zone. Martin (2002) still claims the presence of a vitreous space, although a much smaller one than earlier authors.
Schewiakoff (1889, as reported by Conant, 1898) held that there are two types of cells in the retina of both the large and small complex eyes, called pigment and visual cells. Conant (1898) himself could not distinguish two types of cells in the retina. Berger (1900) found three distinct cell types in the large complex eye, which he termed: prism cells and pyramid cells (both presumed sensory), and long pigment cells (presumed structural). Here, he draws an analogy between the prism and pyramid cells and the rods and cones of vertebrates. However, he found only one type of cell, the prism cell, in the small complex eye (Berger, 1900). Ng (1974) reports four types of cells in the retina of the large complex eyes: long and short pigmented cells, and type A and B receptor cells. Laska and Hündgen (1982) report three types of cells in the large, and two in the small, complex eyes. The number and type of cells reported in the retina differ largely from author to author and it is not useful to go into the details of each of the cell types beyond this. Rather I will say this is an area where careful revisitaiton will lead to much clarification. Obviously there is conflicting information here and not all these descriptions can be accurate. This variation likely comes from collection, fixation, or microscopy techniques all of which have improved greatly since these earliest descriptions.
The Photoreceptors
The photoreceptors of cubozoans, and all cnidarians, are ciliary with the 9+2 arrangement of microtubules (Yamasu and Yoshida, 1976). They are made up of a receptive outer segment, a pigmented layer, and a nuclear layer. Many microvilli extend from the central cilium (Yamasu and Yoshida, 1976) and these are thought to be involved in primary photoreception (Laska and Hündgen, 1982; Takasu and Yoshida, 1984). The photoreceptors themselves contain the screening pigment (Laska and Hündgen, 1982). Martin (2002) reports that the outer segments possess stacks of parallel membranes which form discs similar to vertebrate rod cells. These cells contain many mitorchondria and vesicles (Martin, 2002). An axon leaves from the nuclear side. Connections from these photoreceptors lead to the rest of the nervous system through interneurons (Laska and Hündgen, 1982).
NERVOUS SYSTEM ORGANIZATION
In 1898, Conant recognized the presence of the nerve ring as distinguishing cubomedusae from scyphomedusae. The nerve ring connects between the rhopalia and the pedalia (points of attachment of the tentacles) (Conant, 1898; Laska and Hündgen, 1984). The nervous system can be divided into three functional parts: the sensory clubs or rhopalia, the conducting nerve ring, and the motor nerve net underlying the subumbrellar epithelium (Conant, 1898; Passano, 1982). In the course of the nerve ring there are several sets of ganglia: four radial ganglia situated where the nerve ring branches to connect to the rhopalia and four pedal ganglia situated where the nerve ring sends off nerves to the tentacles (Conant, 1898; Laska and Hündgen, 1984).
The rhopalia are filled with nerve fibers and cell bodies, referred to as a rhopalial ganglion. This ganglion fills most of the space between the sensory organs and the extension of the gut which enters the rhopalium (Conant, 1898; Ng, 1974; Laska and Hündgen, 1984). There are two groups of large multipolar ganglion cells, one above each pit ocelli. The area between and behind the small complex eye is filled with a large group of network cells (Berger, 1900). The tissue directly below the retina is composed of ganglion cells and nerve fibers (Conant, 1898; Berger, 1900). These form synaptic connections (Laska and Hündgen, 1982) via interneurons to the rhopalial ganglion cells (Yamasu and Yoshida, 1976).
Removing the rhopalia completely leads to a cessation of swimming (Berger, 1900). Satterlie (1979, 2002) has shown that the swim pacemaker cells reside in the rhopalial ganglia. Unfortunately, due to the location of these pacemaker cells, and the close association of the rhopalial ganglia with the eyes, it is impossible to do the desired experiment of removing the rhopalial ganglion and leaving the eyes and swimming behavior intact. If this experiment were possible I would predict that removing the rhopalial ganglion cells would eliminate a behavioral response to light even if the eyes and the swim system remained unharmed. It is my opinion that the location of the rhopalial ganglion cells leaves them poised to sort information encoded by the eyes and to dictate appropriate behavioral responses.
Cephalization may not be appropriate for a radially symmetrical animal
It is easy to be biased by bilateral symmetry and assume that a centralized nervous system is necessary for any integrative nervous system function. However, this may not only be unnecessary but actually detrimental in a radially symmetrical animal, where sensory input comes from several directions. Spencer and Arkett (1984) have suggested, based on work in the hydromedusae Polyorchis, that: 1) “the nerve-rings constitute the central nervous system of hydrozoan jellyfish and that the condensation of neuronal networks into ring structures is the maximum degree of localization that can be tolerated, bearing in mind the integrative mechanisms used,” 2) “networks of identical neurones are the radiates' equivalent of identifiable, paired neurones in the CNS of bilateral protostomes” and 3) “rather than cephalic condensation …the hydromedusae have distributed their ganglia throughout the outer nerve-ring so as to match the distributed arrangement of mulitcellular sense organs such as the ocelli.” These ideas are supported by other authors for a host of cnidarian groups (Passano, 1976; Satterlie, 2002; Mackie, 2002). These arguments hold for the cubomedusan nervous system as well.
The function of the nervous system is to sense the environment and respond appropriately (Satterlie and Nolen, 2001). Cubomedusae have their sensory systems located around the margin of the bell where they can receive information from all directions. They have their integrative centers (rhopalial ganglia) concentrated near these sensory inputs (Satterlie, 1979; Satterlie and Spencer, 1979; Laska and Hündgen, 1984). It is known that the swim pacemakers located in the rhopalial ganglia communicate with eachother via the nerve ring (Sattelie and Nolen, 2001). These rhopalial ganglia presumably sort this information and then distribute it to the appropriate effectors, the swim musculature, via the distributed nerve net of the subumbrella. Distributed sense organs make the most sense in a radially symmetric organism (Satterlie and Spencer, 1987; Satterlie, 2002).
Satterlie and Nolen (2001) have shown that the reduction to only four swim pacemakers in cubomedusae, compared to the higher multiples of four seen in scyphomedusae, represents a partial of centralization of the cubozoan nervous system. These swim pacemakers are located in the rhopalia and allow for rapid responses to asymetrical stimuli (Satterlie and Nolen, 2001). This accounts for the agile swimming ability and rapid directional changes seen in cubomedusae (Satterlie and Nolen, 2001). Could this be in response to visual information?
PREVIOUS IDEAS FOR THE ROLE OF THE COMPLEX EYES IN A BOX JELLYFISH
Lenses not only focus light but they also increase light capture. Some authors have suggested that this may be the sole purpose of the complex eyes (Laska and Hündgen, 1982; Stewart, 1996). However, there are no known examples of animals that employ this strategy (Nilsson, personal communication). Another idea that has been put forth is that the complex eyes allow for orientation to luminescent prey at night (Pearse and Pearse, 1978). Matsumoto (1996) suggests that vision is primarily involved in feeding and reproduction (see also Pearse and Pearse, 1978; Martin, 2002). These two tasks can also be carried out with chemoreception. Several authors have suggested some sort of near-sighted view of small objects inside the bell (Berger, 1898; Conant, 1898; Pearse and Pearse, 1978; Laska and Hündgen, 1984; Stewart, 1996; Matsumoto, 1996) because of the inward orientation of the eyes. However, viewing small objects inside the bell is also unlikely. The optics of the lenses of both the large and small complex eyes show long focal lengths (Coates et al., 2001). This means the lens acts as a low pass filter. A low pass filter removes high spatial frequency image information (such as small objects) before that information reaches the retina. This leaves only low frequency image information (larger objects such as mangrove roots) for the retina to detect. This will be the ideal use for the complex eyes. A low pass filter will give enough information to avoid obstacles in the water. Also, transparency of the bell allows for possibly unencumbered view of the outside world (Stewart, 1996). Meaning that the inward pointing orientation of the complex eyes will not restrict the field of view to objects within the bell.
THE LIFE HISTORY AND ECOLOGY OF THE CUBOMEDUSAE DIFFERS FROM OTHER MEDUSAE
Most medusae are open ocean drifters
Most medusae are found far from shore and are planktonic, at the mercy of the currents and not capable of swimming against them (Graham et al., 2001). In many areas it is uncommon to see medusae near shore unless they have been swept in by changing currents and tides. Often in these cases, medusae are washed up on shore and die.
Noteably, all epipelagic, midwater, and deepsea medusae have very simple, reduced, or absent ocelli. For example, all species in the order Narcomedusae lack ocelli (Mayer, 1910). Species of narcomedusae are found from the surface down to depths greater than 1,000 m (Raskoff, 2001). While we think of jellyfish as predators, their method of food capture more closely resembles fishing than hunting. Beyond predator and prey locations, or conspecific interactions, the open ocean offers little environmental visual information. Still, with simple ocelli, openwater medusae perform phototactic behaviors. For example, Aurelia aurita (Scyphozoa) navigates by sun-compass (Hamner et al., 1994). Many hydrozoan species perform large vertical diel migrations, which may bring individuals together in spawning aggregations (Mills, 1983). Even with large migrations, open ocean jellyfish rarely encounter obstacles in their environments.
Cubomedusae are neritic, often living in complex near-shore habitats such as mangrove swamps
A note on taxonomy: the number and descriptions of species has changed many times over the years (Haeckel, 1879; Mayer, 1910; Bigelow, 1918, 1938; Stiasny, 1930; Kramp, 1961; Uchida, 1970) and there is a great need to revisit this issue to form some consensus. For the discussion below, however, I am interested in conveying the typical habitat of the cubomedusae. To this end I will use the species names and spellings used by the authors who collected and remarked on the habitat of a certain cubozoan.
Cubomedusae are near-shore species, usually tropical or sub-tropical (Werner, 1973). Agassiz and Mayer (1902) found one individual, Carybdea grandis in a trawl that came from a depth of 300 fathoms during the expedition of the ‘Albatross,’ 1899–1900. This may have contributed to the early misconception that cubomedusae were deepsea animals. However, the very next day they found a swarm of mature adults very near the surface off Anaa Island.
Reports of cubomedusae date back more than 200 years (Linne, 1758). While the exact habitats are not always recorded, we are beginning to get a sense of their natural history, and they appear to prefer near-shore habitats, such as mangroves, kelp forests, coral reefs, and sandy beaches (Table 1).
The cubozoan Tripedalia cystophora actively swims near the surface during the day (Conant, 1898; Stewart, 1996), maintaining position among the mangrove roots without damaging its delicate body. Cubozoans are agile swimmers (Stewart, 1996) and voracious feeders (Larson, 1976).
DISCUSSION
By bringing together all the work of previous authors, whether it be on life history or nervous system function, a picture emerges that details the interactions of cubozoans with their environments. From this picture it is now possible to make predictions about vision in the Cubozoa.
SUPPOSITION: SPATIAL VISION ALLOWS FOR EXPLOITATION OF PRODUCTIVE NEAR-SHORE HABITATS BY CUBOZOANS
As seen in Table 1, all known species of cubomedusae are found in near-shore habitats. Have the eyes of the cubomedusae allowed them to expand into this ecological niche?
Presupposition: medusae are soft, delicate, and easily damaged by obstacles in the water column
For many years these beautiful creatures were incompletely described because what came up in plankton nets was often severely damaged. Improved collecting techniques increased knowledge over the past century, but still laboratory observations of behavior were often short-term before the invention of the planktonkriesel (Greve, 1968; Hamner, 1990; Raskoff et al., 2003). This specialized tank creates a waterflow that keeps medusae and other delicate animals in constant suspension assuring that their tissues are not destroyed by abrasive contact with aquarium walls. Can an animal with such a delicate body survive a near-shore habitat, where obstacles form effective walls? Cubomedusae live in such environments but they must have some way to keep from colliding with such obstacles.
Presupposition: vision is a useful way to detect objects at a distance
Chemoreception lacks the resolution needed to avoid obstacles. Mechanoreception will not give information on a time scale which leaves the medusae able to respond; once contact is made the damage has been done. Given the right resolution, spatial vision will allow for detection and avoidance of obstacles.
Counter-supposition: cubozoan eyes represent evolution gone wild, form without function
Gerhart and Kirschner (1997) propose that the cubozoan eyes shows how far form can go without function; citing the lack of a brain as evidence against the usefulness of these eyes. In fact, most skepticism about the image forming capabilities of the box jellyfish eye comes in this form: what good is an image without the nervous ability to interepret it (Nilsson and Pelger, 1994; Darwin, 1872)? Mackie (1999) is quick to point out the under appreciation of, and in fact often misinformation concerning, the cnidarian nervous system (see also Passano, 1976; Mackie, 2002; Satterlie, 2002). Everyone agrees that cnidarians can respond to mechanical and chemical stimulation, should visual information be fundamentally different?
Although cubomedusae are a somewhat neglected group more is being learned about them all the time. When compared to other meduseae we see sophisticated eye design, complex nervous systems, and a unique ecological niche. Bringing together the knowledge of previous researchers we are poised to explore the role of vision in the Cubozoa.
Table 1. Summary of cubozoan species with reported habitats
Open in new tab
Table 1. Summary of cubozoan species with reported habitats
Open in new tab
Open in new tabDownload slide
Fig. 1. A. A schematic diagram of the rhopalium. The large and small complex eyes lie along the medial line while the pit and slit ocelli are paired laterally. B. Drawing of Tripedalia cystophora courtesy of F. Sommer. Note that T. cystophora is unusual among cubozoans in having a rounded, not box-shaped bell. Each medusa has four rhopalia located around the margin of the bell (arrows) which alternate with the four groups of tentacles. The complex eyes point toward the center of the bell
1 From the Symposium Comparative and Integrative Vision Research presented at the Annual Meeting of the Society for Integrative and Comparative Biology, 4–8 January 2003, at Toronto, Canada.
2 Present address: Melissa Coates c/o Department of Cell and Organism Biology, Lund University, Helgonavägen 3, Lund, Sweden, 223 62; E-mail: honu@stanford.edu
I would like to thank Jamie Theobald for support, advice, and useful discussion, as well as Stuart Thompson and Dan-Eric Nilsson. I thank Caren Braby and Christian Reilly for careful reading of this manuscript as well as the two anonymous reviewers. Thanks to Mason Posner for inviting me to participate in the symposium. Thanks to Mason Posner, Todd Oakley, and Sonke Johnson for organizing the symposium.
References
Agassiz A.
1902
. Reports on the scientific results of the expedition to the tropical pacific in charge of Alexander Agassiz by the US Fish Commission steamer ‘Albatross,’ from August 1899, to March, 1900, Commander Jefferson F. Moser, USN, commanding. III. Medusae. XXVI 3, Mem. Mus. comp. Zool. Harv.
Arneson A.
1976
. Life history of Carybdea alata Reynaud, 1830 (CUBOMEDUSAE). In G. O. Mackie (ed.), Coelenterate ecology and behavior, pp. 227–236. Plenum Press, New York.
Barnes J.
1966
. Studies on three venemous cubomedusae. In W. Rees (ed.), The Cnidaria and their evolution, pp. 307–332. Academic Press, New York.
Berger E.
1898
. The histological structure of the eyes of cubomedusae.
J. Comp. Neurol
,
8
223
-230.
Berger E.
1900
. Physiology and histology of the Cubomedusae, including Dr. F.S. Conant's notes on the physiology.
Mem. Biol. Lab. Johns Hopkins Univ
, IV
1
-84.
Bigelow H.
1909
. The Medusae. Mem. Mus. Comp. Zool. Harv XXXVII(XVI).
Bigelow H.
1918
. Some medusae and siphonophorae from the western Atlantic.
Bull. Mus. Comp. Zool. Harv
,
8
365
-442.
Bigelow H.
1938
. Medusae taken during 1929 and 1930.
Zoologica: N.Y
, XXIII
135
-179.
Claus C.
1878
. Ueber Charybdea marsupialis.
Arb. Zool. Inst. Univ. Wien
, II
16
-55.
Coates M.
2000
. Vision in a cubozoan jellyfish.
Amer. Zool
,
40
977
.
Coates M.
2001
. The image forming capabilities of the camera-type eyes of a box jellyfish, Tripedalia cystophora.
Amer. Zool
,
41
1640
.
Conant F.
1898
. The cubomedusae.
Mem. Biol. Lab. Johns Hopkins Univ
, IV
1
-61.
Darwin C.
1872
. The origin of species. 6th ed. Hill and Wang, New York.
Fenner F.
1987
. Experiments with the nematocysts of Carybdea rastoni (‘Jimble’). Med. J. Aus. 147.
Fenner P.
1985
. ‘Morbakka,’ another cubomedusan.
Med. J. Aus
,
143
550
-555.
Gerhart J.
1997
. Cells embryos and evolution. Blackwell Science, Malden, Massachusetts.
Graham W.
2001
. A physical context for gelatinous zooplankton aggregations: a review.
Hydrobiologia
,
452
199
-212.
Greve W.
1968
. The ‘planktonkreisel’, a new device for culturing zooplankton.
Mar. Biol
,
1
201
-203.
Haeckel E.
1879
. Des System der Medusen: erster Theil einer Monographie der Medusen, pp. 423–449. Jena.
Hamner W.
1990
. Design developments in the planktonkriesel, a plankton aquarium for ships at sea.
J. Plankton Res
,
12
397
-402.
Hamner W.
1994
. Sun-compass migration by Aurelia aurita (Scyphozoa): Population retention and reproduction in Saanich Inlet, British Columbia.
Mar. Biol
,
119
347
-356.
Hamner W.
1995
. Swimming, feeding, circulation, and vision in the Australian box jellyfish, Chironex fleckeri (Cnidaria: Cubozoa).
Mar. Fresh. Res
,
46
985
-990.
Hartwick R.
1991
. Observations on the anatomy, behaviour, reproduction and life cycle of the cubozoan it Carybdea sivickisi.
Hydrobiologia
,
216
/217
171
-179.
Hoverd W.
1985
. Occurrence of the order Cubomedusae (Cnidaria: Scyphozoa) in New Zealand: Collectian and laboratory observations of Carybdea sivickisi.
New Zealand J. Zool
,
12
107
-110.
Kramp P.
1961
. Syonopsis of the medusae of the world.
J. Mar. Biol. Assn. U.K
,
40
304
-310.
Larson R.
1976
. Cubomedusae: Feeding—functional morphology, behavior and phylogenetic position. In G. O. Mackie (ed.), Coelenterate ecology and behavior, pp. 237–245. Plenum Press, New York.
Larson R.
1990
. Two medusae new to the coast of California: Carybdea marsupialis (Linneaus 1758), a cubomedusa and Phyllorhiza punctata von Lendenfeld, 1884, a rhizostome scyphomedusa.
Bull. Sth. Calif. Acad. Sci
,
89
130
-136.
Laska G.
1982
. Morphologie und ultrastruktur der lichtsinnesorgane von Tripedalia cystophora Conant (Cnidaria, Cubozoa).
Zool. Jb. Anat
,
108
107
-123.
Laska G.
1984
. Die ultrastruktur des neuromuskulären systems der medusen von Tripedalia cystophora und Carybdea marsupialis (Coelenterata, Cubozoa).
Zoomorphology
,
104
163
-170.
Laska-Mehnert G.
1985
. Cytologishe veränderungen während der metamorphose des cubopolypen Tripedalia cystophora (Cubozoa, Carybdeidae) in die medusae. Helgoländer wiss.
Meeresunters
,
39
129
-164.
Light S.
1921
. Further notes on Philippine scyphomedusaen jellyfishes.
Philipp. J. Sci
,
18
25
-45.
Linne C.
1758
. Systamae Naturae. .
Mackie G.
1999
. Coelenterate organs.
Mar. Fresh. Behav. Physiol
,
32
113
-127.
Mackie G.
2002
. What's new in cnidarian biology?
Can. J. Zool
,
80
1649
-1653.
Martin V.
2002
. Photoreceptors of cnidarians.
Can. J. Zool
,
80
1703
-1722.
Matsumoto G.
1996
. Observations on the anatomy and behaviour of the cubozoan Carybdea rastonii.
Haacke. Mar. Fresh. Behav. Physiol
,
26
139
-148.
Mayer A.
1900
. Some Medusae from the Tortugas, Florida. .
Mus. Comp. Zool. Harv
, XXXVII
70
-71.
Mayer A.
1910
. The Hydromedusae. In Medusae of the world. Vol. II.
Mills C. E.
1983
. Vertical migration and diel activity patterns of hydromedusae: Studies in a large tank.
J. Plankton Res
,
5
619
-635.
Moore S.
1988
. A new species of cubomedusan (Cubozoa: Cnidaria) from Northern Australia. Beagle, Rec. North.
Terr. Mus. Arts Sci
,
5
1
-4.
Ng F.
1974
. The Cubomedusae: A review of the literature and histology of the eyes. M.A. Thesis, Victoria Univ. Wellington, New Zealand.
Nilsson D.-E.
1994
. A pessimistic estimate of the time required for an eye to evolve.
Proc. R. Soc. B
,
256
53
-58.
Passano L.
1976
. Strategies for the study of the coelenterate brain. In G. Mackie (ed.), Coelenterate ecology and behavior, pp. 639–645. Plenum Press, New York.
Passano L.
1982
. Scyphozoa and Cubozoa. In G. Shelton (ed.), Electrical conduction and behaviour in ‘simple’ invertebrates, pp. 149–202. Carendon Press, Oxford.
Pearse J.
1978
. Vision in cubomedusan jellyfishes.
Science
,
199
458
.
Piatigorsky J.
1989
. The cellular eye lens and crystallins of cubomedusan jellyfish.
J. Comp. Phys. A
,
164
577
-587.
Piatigorsky J.
1993
. J1-crystallins of the cubomedusan jellyfish lens constitute a novel family encoded in at least three intronless genes.
J. Biol. Chem
,
268
11894
-11901.
Raskoff K.
2001
. The ecology of the mesopelagic hydromedusae in Monterey Bay, California. Ph.D. Diss., University of California, Los Angeles.
Raskoff K.
2003
. Collection and culture techniques for gelatinous zooplankton.
Biol. Bull. Mar. Biol. Lab., Woods Hole
,
204
68
-80.
Satterlie R.
1979
. Central control of swimming in the cubomedusan jellyfish Carybdea rastonii.
J. Comp. Phys. A
,
133
357
-367.
Satterlie R.
2002
. Neuronal control of swimming in jellyfish: A comparative story.
Can. J. Zool
,
80
1654
-1669.
Satterlie R.
2001
. Why do cubomedusae have only four swim pacemakers? .
J. Exp. Biol
,
204
(Pt 8)
1413
-9.
Satterlie R.
1979
. Swimming control in a cubomedusan jellyfish.
Nature
,
281
141
-142.
Satterlie R.
1987
. Organization of conducting systems in ‘simple’ invertebrates: Porifera, Cnidaria, and Ctenophora. In M. Ali (ed.), Nervous systems in invertebrates, pp. 213–264. Plenum Press, New York.
Schewiakoff W.
1889
. Beitrage zur Xenntniss des Acalephenauges.
Morph. Jahrb
, XV
21
-60.
Southcott R.
1956
. Studies on Australian cubomedusae, including a new genus and species apparently harmful to man.
Mar. Fresh. Res
,
7
254
-280.
Southcott R.
1967
. Revision of some Carybdeidae (Scyphozoa: Cubozoa), including a description of the jellyfish responsible for the ‘Irukandji Syndrome.’.
Australian J. Zool
,
15
651
-671.
Spencer A.
1984
. Radial symmetry and the organization of central neurones in a hydrozoan jellyfish.
J. Exp. Biol
,
110
69
-90.
Stewart S.
1996
. Field behavior of Tripedalia cystophora (Class Cubozoa).
Mar. Fresh. Behav. Physiol
,
27
175
-188.
Stiasny G.
1930
. Scyphomedusen. Résultats Scientifiques du Voyage aux Indes Orientales Neerlandaises de LL. AA. RR. le Prince et la Princesse Leopold de Belgique.
Mem. Mus. R. Hist. Nat. Belg
, II
1
-12.
Studabaker J.
1972
. Development of the cubomedusa, Carybdea marsupialis. M.S. Thesis, University of Puerto Rico, Mayagüez.
Takasu N.
1984
. Freeze-fracture and histofluorescence studies on photoreceptive membranes of medusan ocelli.
Zool. Sci
,
1
367
-374.
Uchida T.
1970
. Revision of Japanese Cubomedusae.
Pub. Seto Mar. Biol. Lab
, XVII
289
-297.
Werner B.
1971
. Life cycle of Tripedalia cystophora Conant (Cubomedusae).
Nature
,
232
582
-583.
Werner B.
1973
. New investigation on sytematics and evolution of the class Scyphozoa and the phylum Cnidaria. In Recent Trends in Research in Coelenterate Biology, pp. 35–59.
Werner B.
1975
. Bau und lebensgeschichte des polypen von Tripedalia cystophora (Cubozoa, class. nov., Carybdeidae) und seine bedeutung fur die evolution der Cnidaria. Helgoländer wiss.
Meeresunters
,
27
461
-504.
Werner B.
1976
. Muscular and nervous system of the cubopolyp (Cnidaria).
Experientia
,
32
1047
-1049.
Yamasu T.
1976
. Fine structure of complex ocelli of a cubomedusan, Tamoya bursaria Haeckel. .
Cell Tissue Res
,
170
325
-39.
Yatsu N.
1917
. Notes on the physiology of Charybdea rastonii.
J. Coll. Sci. Imp. Univ. Tokyo
, XL
1
-12.
The Society for Integrative and Comparative Biology
Issue Section:
Regular Article
Download all slides
Advertisement
Citations
Views
9,314
Altmetric
More metrics information
Email alerts
Article activity alert
Advance article alerts
New issue alert
Receive exclusive offers and updates from Oxford Academic
Citing articles via
Google Scholar
-
Latest
-
Most Read
-
Most Cited
More from Oxford Academic
Biological Sciences
Science and Mathematics
Books
Journals
Advertisement